+ All Categories
Home > Documents > FullText (18)

FullText (18)

Date post: 06-Jul-2018
Category:
Upload: ham-fg
View: 219 times
Download: 0 times
Share this document with a friend

of 65

Transcript
  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    1/65

    TitleAn investigation on the invented spellings of grade one childrenaged between 5-6 years old in a Hong Kong primary school

    Author(s) Ho, Civen;OU`z

    Citation

    Issued Date 2015

    URL http://hdl.handle.net/10722/223647

    Rights Creative Commons: Attribution 3.0 Hong Kong License

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    2/65

    An investigation on the inventedspellings of grade one children

    aged between 5-6 years old in a

    Hong Kong primary school

    By: Civen Ho

    BEd. Language Education (Primary English),

    The University of Hong Kong

    A research dissertation

    A classroom-based research project submitted in partial fulfillment

    of the requirements for the Master of Education in Early ChildhoodEducation at The University of Hong Kong

    31st August 2015

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    3/65

      2

    Declaration

    I hereby declare that this dissertation is a representation of my own work, and it has not

     been previously submitted to this University or any other institution in application to a

    degree, diploma or other qualifications.

    Signature:

     Name: Civen Ho

    UID: 2010527587

    Date: 31st August 2015

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    4/65

      3

    !"#$%&'()*(+($,-

    If this research dissertation were to be described using a metaphor, I would say that it was

    fairly similar to a roller coaster ride. There were many moments where I experienced

    highs and lows, ups and downs. This research dissertation was truly an unforgettable ride.

    Firstly, I would like to express my deepest gratitude to my supervisor Ms. Diana Lee for

    her never ending support throughout this year. I deeply appreciated the guidance and

    constructive feedback from the very beginning of this research. It would not have been

    made possible without your continued support. I truly appreciated the constructive

    feedback, which has allowed for improvement during the writing of this research

    dissertation.

    Secondly, I would like to thank the school and research participants who were kind

    enough to agree to participate in this study. The contributions they have made to this

    research dissertation has been invaluable. It would not have been made possible without

    them.

    Lastly, I would like to thank my family for their continued love and support during the

    writing of this research dissertation.

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    5/65

      4

    !/-,01",The research conducted can be classified as an intrinsic case study, which sought to

    examine and analyse the invented spellings made by grade one children in primary school

    within the Hong Kong context. The research hopes to develop a better understanding of

    the common errors made by grade one children and the spelling development progress

    they underwent. One grade one class was chosen to participate in this research. They

    were required to provide their writing summative assessments they had completed during

    the course of the school year. There were four writing summative assessments for each

    child. In total, eighty samples were collected and five hundred and forty eight words were

    identified as invented spellings. The invented spellings were then given a score from a

    10-point scale to determine the type of error that was made. From the results, it suggests

    that children’s spelling development is not fixed to a specific level, and that the spelling

    errors made can be attributed to the phonological differences between Cantonese and

    English.

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    6/65

      5

    21/'( %3 "%$,($,-

    4516,(0 7 8$,0%)9",:%$Page number

    91.1 Contextual background 9

    1.2 Research rationale and foci 10

    4516,(0 ; :(& 132.1 Definition of invented spelling 13

    2.2 Stages of spelling/invented spelling2.2.1

     

    Prephonetic stage

    2.2.2  Phonetic stage2.2.3  Transitional stage

    2.2.4 

    Conventional stage

    1313

    1415

    162.3 Linguistic description of English and

    Chinese

    17

    2.4 Component processes involved in invented

    spelling2.4.1  Phonemic awareness

    2.4.2  Working memory2.4.3

     

    Orthographic awareness

    2.4.4  Oral vocabulary2.4.5  Knowledge of morphology

    17

    18

    1919

    2021

    2.5 

    Benefits of invented spelling2.5.1  Improves conventional spelling

    2.5.2  Improves reading skill2.5.3  Improves writing skill

    2122

    2223

    2.6 The case of Chinese ESL learners and

    spelling/invented spelling2.7 Phonological differences between

    Cantonese and English

    23

    25

    4516,(0 ? @(,5%)%'%*A 303.1 Research questions 303.2 Methodological approach 30

    3.3 Research setting 303.4 Participants 30

    3.5 Procedure 313.6 Ethical considerations 32

    3.7 Data analysis methods 323.7.1 Spelling sophistication: Scoring

    method

    32

    3.7.2 Categorization of invented

    spellings

    34

    4516,(0 B C:$):$*- 354.1  Number of invented spellings across the 35

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    7/65

      6

    two terms4.2

     

    Spelling sophistication in the first term 36

    4.3 Spelling sophistication in the second term 394.4 Comparison of spelling sophistication in

    the first and second term

    42

    4.5 

    Invented spelling errors made in the firstterm 43

    4.6 Invented spelling errors made in the

    second term

    46

    4516,(0 D @(,5%)%'%*A 495.1

     

    Common spelling errors made by grade

    one children

    49

    5.2 Comparison of invented spelling errors in

    the first and second term

    51

    5.3 Implications 53

    4516,(0 E 4%$"'9-:%$ 546.1 Key issues and insights 54

    6.2 Limitations 546.3 Scope for further research 55

    =(3(0($"(- 56

    !66($):"(- 641.  Sample of Spelling Sophistication Scoring

    Method

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    8/65

      7

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    9/65

      8

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    10/65

      9

    4516,(0 7F 8$,0%)9",:%$

    7G7 4%$,(H,91' I1"#*0%9$)

    The acquisition of literacy was always thought to follow a specific sequence, where you

    learn to read, spell and then write. However, Chomsky (1971) has suggested that it was

     possible for children to learn basic spelling strategies before they were able to read. At an

    early age, children already begin to interact with written language as they attempt to

    represent words in print. When they start experimenting with the written code, they can

     be seen to create their own spellings. While these spellings may stray far from the

    conventional spelling, they should not be overlooked. These early pre-conventional

    spellings are often referred to as invented spelling. Invented spelling is used to describe a

    child’s first efforts in spelling words, which they have yet to master (Cramer, 1998).

    Through invented spellings, children are able to create written versions of words based on

    the letter-sound relationships that they know, and represent their interpretations of how a

    word is spelled based on how it sounds (Savage, 2011). Moreover, Read (1975) states

    that children are capable of spelling a word based on “how it is articulated or felt in their

    mouths” (He & Wang, 2009, p. 45). This can be seen as an indication that they have

     begun to develop awareness to the internal structure of words, in particular, awareness tothe phonemic segments that are represented by the alphabet (Tangel & Blachman, 1992).

    Moreover, children start to analyse the phonological components of words and access the

     phonemic structure of words, which can only be gained through writing or formal

    teaching (Pontecorvo & Orsolini, 1996; Ferreiro, 1991; Vernon, 1998 as cited in Silva &

    Martins, 2003). Thereby carrying the implication that invented spelling is related to the

    child’s phonological awareness and product of how particular grapheme-phoneme

     principles are used (Treiman, Tincoff, & Richmond-Welty, 1996). Children gradually

    reach the point of conventional spelling as they fine-tune their productions as they learn

    more on how speech is mapped to print and the rules of standard English orthography

    (Tangel & Blachman, 1992).

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    11/65

      10

    According to Gentry (2000) invented spellings can be considered to be a window, which

    “allows the observer to assess and teach not only spelling, but also important aspects of

     phonemic awareness, phonics, writing and other essential elements of literacy” (p. 318).

    Since it reveals the writer’s awareness of the relationship that exists among the speech

    sounds and the principles regulating the correspondences between graphemes and

     phonemes (He & Wang, 2009). In order to ensure that children are able to become

    competent spellers, it is essential for the teacher to determine, which stage of

    development each child is at (Sipe, 2008 as cited in Westwood, 2014).

    Therefore to better understand the spelling development that occurs in children,

    researchers have examined their spellings from different perspectives, which include

    spelling development based on their second language (L2) acquisition. Children whose

    native language differs from the instructional language used in school can be faced with

    the challenge of mastering skills, such as reading and spelling abilities, in a language they

    have not yet fully acquired (Jongejan, Verhoeven & Siegel, 2007). Since writing systems

    are graphic representations of the spoken language, it helps facilitate literacy

    development, which involves learning the association found between the printed and oral

    forms of the language (Adams, 1990). By observing the development of spelling

    knowledge, it can help to understand and facilitate L2 literacy acquisition.

    7G; =(-(10"5 01,:%$1'( 1$) 3%":

    The Hong Kong government has imposed a language policy, which aims for children, at

    the end of their secondary education, to be biliterate and trilingual. They are expected to

     be proficient in writing Chinese and English, while being able to communicate

    effectively in Chinese, English and Mandarin. Once entering primary school, young

    children begin their formal instruction in becoming proficient language users of Chinese,

    English and Mandarin.

    Many of the children entering school will be learning English as a second language (ESL),

    and have Chinese as their L1. It should be noted that English and Chinese are two

    languages that greatly contrast one another. English is classified as being in the Indo-

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    12/65

      11

    European language family (Yule, 1985, as cited in Chan & Li, 2000). It is an alphabetic

    script and is considered to be orthographically deep due to the complexity of the letter-

    sound relationships. In comparison, Chinese is in the Sino-Tibetan language family (Li

    and Thompson, 1981, as cited in Chan & Li, 2000). The writing system is considered to

     be morphosyllabic since the characters each represent a monosyllabic morpheme

    (Marinova-Todd & Hall, 2013).

    Several studies have suggested that bilingual speakers are capable of transferring the

    required skills for literacy development from one language to another (Gottardo et al.,

    2001; Sparks, Patton, Ganschow, Humbach, & Janorksy, 2008). It has been suggested

    that the processes linked to the development of English literacy skills are similar in

    monolingual and bilingual children (Marinova-Todd & Hall, 2013). However, the

    differences in the Chinese and English writing system has lead some researchers to

    question whether the phonological awareness skills in English have the same important

    role in Chinese literacy development.

    Phonological awareness plays a crucial part in helping children learn to read and spell

    since it helps one understand the language’s sound structure and includes the ability to

    segment speech and detect and manipulate phonemes (Jongejan, Verhoeven & Siegel,

    2007). Since studies have shown the transfer of phonological awareness skills from one

    language another, it would be beneficial to examine ESL children’s invented spellings in

    order to deepen understanding and provide further insight on their spelling development.

    The school chosen for the research has established a positive and encouraging learning

    environment for children to utilize invented spellings in their writing. They are given

    many opportunities to practice and develop their spellings through using invented

    spellings. The findings of this study will contribute to the perspective of an ESL’s

    invented spellings and understanding on young children’s spelling development

     particularly in invented spellings as they progress through their first year in primary

    school.

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    13/65

      12

    The present study investigated the invented spelling errors made by grade one children by

    tracking their invented spellings in their writing summative assessment across the school

    year. The writing summative assessments approximately occurred every two to three

    months.

    Objectives:

    1.  To identify the common errors in invented spelling made by the grade one

    children.

    2.  To compare the invented spelling errors made between the first and second term.

    In the next chapters, I will look further into the literature gap identified, which is the

    development of ESL children’s invented spellings over their first year in primary school.

    I will explain in greater detail how this research examined and tracked ESL children’s

    invented spellings over their first year. 

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    14/65

      13

    4516,(0 ;F :(&

    This chapter will explore the nature of invented spelling by examining the stages of

    spelling, and highlighting the component processes needed. Particular attention will be

     paid to the spelling development of Chinese ESL learners, who share the same first andsecond language as the participants chosen for the study. All of these will be covered in

    hopes to enhance understanding of invented spelling among young children and

    especially in Chinese ESL learners. In addition, this will be referred to in later chapters to

    guide and facilitate discussion on the invented spellings analysed.

    ;G7 J(3:$:,:%$ %3 :$>($,() -6('':$*

    Invented spelling is used to describe a child’s early attempts at spelling, where they

    essentially make up their own spellings for words that they do not know or remember the

    conventional spelling (Ott, 2007). It is the result of the writer trying to apply what they

    understand about the internalized principles of grapheme-phoneme associations to their

    spelling of words they have not mastered yet (Gentry, 2000; Read, 1971, 1975 as cited in

    He & Wang, 2009). Invented spelling is a normal part of the child’s on-going

    development as a writer and acts as a powerful indicator on their literacy awareness

    (Savage, 2011).

    ;G; K,1*(- %3 -6('':$*

    Spelling is a developmental process, where as children develop their ability to write, they

     progress through the different stages of spelling development. There are four broad stages

    of spelling development that have been identified accordingly, which are prephonetic,

     phonetic, transitional, and conventional (Savage, 2011).

    !"!"# %&'()*+',-. /,01'

    The prephonetic stage is where children begin to develop the concepts crucial to literacysince they begin to “draw pictures, imitate writing, and begin to learn some letter names

    and forms” (Savage, 2011, p. 158). Within this stage, children’s writing and spelling

    capabilities include scribbling and random spelling (Savage, 2011). At a glance their

    efforts may be overlooked, however their efforts will soon take on characteristics

    resembling features of written English (Cramer, 1998).

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    15/65

      14

    At the age of three years old, children soon develop a basic awareness between the form

    and function of print, where they begin to understand the differences between drawing

    and writing (Savage, 2011). While they are aware of the relationship between writing and

    spoken language, they are not yet aware of the alphabetic principle (Savage, 2011). With

    more exposure to print they gradually enter random spelling, where their writing is absent

    of letter-sound connections. Initially their writings appear to be random scribbles, but

    soon mimic the shapes of written language with the presence of “curves, circles, straight

    lines, dots, joining strokes” (Cramer, 1998, p. 12). Towards the end of this stage, children

    soon start to learn how to read and some sound-symbol relationships become apparent in

    their writing (Savage, 2011).

    !"!"! %)*+',-. /,01'

    At the phonetic stage, children learn to apply the alphabetic principle and more letter

    sound correspondences emerge in their writing as they become more aware of the

    relationship between sounds and letters (Savage, 2011). Children’s spelling is driven by a

     phonetic strategy, which illustrate their capability to make systematic connections

     between letters and sounds. This stage initially starts with the semiphonetic attempts that

    children make in their spelling, where the initial or first couple of letters are included,

    which are then followed by a string of random letters (Savage, 2011). According to

    Henderson (1990), there are two factors influencing children at this stage as phonetic

    spellers, which are the letter name spelling strategy and influence of surrounding speech

    sounds.

    The letter name strategy describes the child’s capability in determining, which letter best

    represents the sound that they hear in the word they would like to spell. They are armed

    with their knowledge of the alphabet principle and awareness that sounds are represented

    with letters, which results in their attempts to apply the names of the letters to the sounds

    (Invernizzi, Abouzeid, and Gill, 1994). Consonant letters frequently appear in their

    spellings as they have letter names that resemble their sounds, as well as long vowel

    sounds, which are identical to their letter names (Cramer, 1998).

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    16/65

      15

    In terms of surrounding speech sounds, Read (1975) has shown that based on the context

    in which letters and sounds appear in within the word determines whether they will be

    included or omitted in the word the children want to spell. An example would be the

    letters m and n, which are often found to be omitted before a final consonant so when the

    word band  is spelled, it may be spelled as bad  (Cramer, 1998).

    However, this does not ensure that children are able to spell the word correctly according

    to the sounds of the given words. Since to represent the long vowel sounds correctly they

    often need two letters, which with the letter name strategy, children would represent the

    long vowel sound in coat as o  instead of oa  (Cramer, 1998). In addition, short vowel

    sounds pose as a problem for young children, and accuracy does not come until much

    later. Nevertheless as they progress through the later stages and develop their initial

    reading skills, they are able to spell more written words that match to conventional

    spelling (Savage, 2011).

    !"!"2 3&0+/-,-*+04 /,01'

    The transitional stage demonstrates children’s increasing awareness that there is a more

    complex nature towards the English orthographic system. They are able to fairly

    consistently use consonants and vowels in their spelling and move past the one-to-one

    letter-sound correspondences (Savage, 2011). This stage can be broken down even

    further into two separate stages, which include within-word pattern, and syllable juncture.

    Within-word pattern is a stage demonstrating children’s understanding that spelling is not

    merely about the one-to-one match of letter to sound. There are a few key indicators that

    demonstrate children’s progress away from the phonetic stage. One of which includes the

    emergence of silent vowel markers, where the “long vowels are ‘marked’ or ‘signaled’ by

    the presence of a silent vowel letter” (Cramer, 1998, p. 20). They are now progressing

    towards spelling with long vowels correctly or “marking” the vowel (Cramer, 1998).

    Another progression is in spelling correctly using short vowel patterns, consonant blends

    and digraphs.

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    17/65

      16

    At the syllable juncture stage children are beginning “to understand how single syllables

    are combined into polysyllabic words” (Savage, 2011). At the syllable juncture, where

    syllables meet, letters are dropped, doubled or changed (Cramer, 1998). This is often

    where most of the spelling errors occur. According to Bear et al. (1996), there are two

    concepts that need to be understood, which are related to adding the suffix  –ed or  –ing .

    The final consonant is doubled in a consonant-vowel-consonant (CVC) pattern, and the

    final e  is dropped in a word with a consonant-vowel-consonant-silent e pattern (CVCe)

    (Bear et al., 1996).

    !"!"5 6*+7'+,-*+04 /,01'

    Children at the conventional spelling stage are gradually approaching to consistently

    applying the standard rules of orthography, where they are aware of the consistencies and

    variations in sound-symbol correspondences (Savage, 2011). They develop awareness to

    derivational relationships, where words similar in meaning also share similar spellings.

    Cunningham (1998) has stated that spelling is what preserves the meaning linkages

    across words. For example the spelling-meaning relationship is demonstrated in the three

    meaning-related words of reside, resident and residential, where the spelling is stable in

    the derived forms even with slight changes in the sound (Cramer, 1998). Thereby

    stressing the importance of vocabulary study so as to enhance their understanding of the

    meaning relationships that exists among words, which would ultimately help them when

    they encounter more polysyllabic words.

    These stages of spelling development can be seen to provide important insights as to how

    learners develop their spelling over time. However, stage-based accounts of spelling

    development is neither static nor fixed as there is considerable overlap in between the

    stages. This has resulted in differing opinions on whether these stage-based accounts of

    spelling development are adequate in portraying an accurate picture of spelling

    development. Treiman and Bourassa (2000) have argued that while they give a rough

    idea of spelling development, it does not fully capture the complexities of phonological

    and morphological representations. Children are in full possession of strategies that are

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    18/65

      17

    available to them at the early stages, which help them and it is not limited to the advanced

    stages of spelling development.

    ;G? () :$ :$>($,() -6('':$*

    Spelling is a complex skill, which “involves the integration of complex perceptual and

    cognitive (brain-based) processes” (Santoro, Coyne and Simmons, 2006 as cited in

    Westwood, 2014, p. 7). Ouellette and Sénéchal (2008) conducted research investigating

    the cognitive and linguistic underpinnings of invented spelling, and have proposed that

     phonemic awareness, working memory, awareness of orthographic rules, oral vocabulary,

    and knowledge of morphology were essential elements that facilitated early spelling

    sophistication. This will be used as a basis in which to explore invented spelling.

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    19/65

      18

    !"5"# %)*+'8-. 090&'+'//

    Phonemic awareness is having the ability to hear, identify and manipulate the sequences

    of basic discrete speech sounds, which are what make spoken words and syllables

    (Savage, 2011). It combines auditory discrimination with the ability to mentally breakdown words and blend the sounds together for the purpose of reading and spelling

    (Westwood, 2014). Moreover, it helps children become aware of how language works as

    it involves them being conscious of the sounds that make up words as being a separate

    entity from the meaning of these words (Savage, 2011).

    Research has shown that phonemic awareness is essential in early reading and spelling

    development (Melby-Lervåg, Lyster and Hulme, 2012; Caravolas, Hulme & Snowling,

    2001; McBride-Chang, 1995). Adams et al. (1998) states that a child’s level of phonemic

    awareness is a strong single determinant on the success of the child’s experience in

    learning to read. Considering that it “primes the reader for print and helps make sense of

     phonics instruction” (Vacca et al.,  2009, p. 148). In addition, Torgesen and Mathes

    (2000) have specified how phonemic awareness is crucial to learning how to read, which

    are due to that it helps children understand the alphabetic principle, how spoken words

    are represented in print, notice how letters represent the sounds in words, be able to

    match letters to sounds when decoding words, and generate possibilities for partially

    sounded out words.

    Moreover, Ouellette and Sénéchal (2008) discovered that phoneme awareness along with

    letter sound knowledge is important predictors for invented spelling in English L1

    children. Yeong and Liow (2011) also found out that it was the strongest predictor for

    spelling sophistication for English L1 children. When young children develop their

    spelling, they need to be able to recognize the consonant and vowel letter sounds in

    words in order to be able to choose the correct letter to spell the words they want to spell,

    and phonemic awareness helps them to achieve this.

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    20/65

      19

    !"5"! :*&;-+1 8'8*&<

    Working memory correlates to short-term memory, and involves holding onto a piece of

    information in the mind, where another piece of information can be added or omitted (Ott,

    2007). Verbal working memory is regarded as the “simultaneous processing, storage andretrieval of verbal information” (Salthouse, 1990 as cited in Keilty & Harrison, 2015, p.

    89). Stage and Wagner (1992) have indicated that working memory resources among

    young monolingual children limits them during spelling tasks. Nevertheless, Verhoeven

    and Siegel (2007) discovered that verbal working memory was an important predictor of

    variance in spelling ability for both lower and upper grade L1 children, but not for ESL

    children.

    !"5"2 =&,)*1&0()-. 090&'+'//

    Orthographic awareness is the correct everyday spelling of a given word, and involves

    studying the spelling to ensure the correct shape and order of the letters (Ott, 2007). For

    children to be able to read or write in English, they need to draw on knowledge of

    orthographic, morphological conventions and phonological knowledge (Treiman, 1993 as

    cited in Marinova-Todd & Hall, 2013). This is further supported by Berninger et al. 

    (1991) who have stated that there are multiple orthographic codes being used along with

     phonological codes as children learn to read and spell. Moreover, in a study conducted byMcBride-Chang (1998), she discovered that in regards to invented spelling, a measure of

    orthographic processing could be used to determine additional variance compared to

    using phonological awareness, letter-name and letter sound knowledge. Her study

    suggests that invented spelling is multifaceted and does not rely merely on

     phonologically based skills but also on orthographic abilities.

    Research studies have brought to light that the differences in orthographic complexity,

     between languages, translates into differences in the development of literacy skills

    (Marinova-Todd & Hall, 2013; Katz & Frost, 1992). Interestingly, there has been

    research suggesting that L1 orthographic characteristics have an influence on L2 reading

    and spelling processes. A longitudinal study conducted by Nassaji (2007) discovered,

    through tracking the developmental changes occurring in a child ESL learner, that there

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    21/65

      20

    was a possibility that his L2 spellings were either influenced or constrained by his L1

    spellings. Moreover, Wang and Geva (2003) reported that Chinese children who were

    ESL learners were found to have poorer spelling performance in spelling pseudowords

    compared to their L1 counterparts. This was explained as being caused by the ESL

    learners reliance on a direct non-phonological route due to their L1, Chinese, having a

    logographic writing system.

    !"5"5 =&04 7*.0>?40&<

    Vocabulary knowledge along with word recognition ability and reading comprehension

    are all interrelated components, which attribute to a comprehensive reading program

    (Rupley, Logan, and Nichols, 1998/99 as cited in Savage, 2011). Vocabulary instruction

    serves a purpose in spelling in that the more information a child knows about a word,

    they will have a greater chance in spelling that word correctly (Savage, 2011). When

    children begin to explore the meaning relations among words and observe their common

    spellings, they will learn the various patterns, in which roots and affixes are combined,

     phonology of English words and how to look for these root forms in words that they

    come across, which will help them learn many new words in one reading and develop

    their own rich vocabulary (Henderson, 1990).

    Raynolds and Uhry (2010) have reported that vocabulary knowledge is “related to the

    ability to spell emerging phonemes for both native English monolingual students and

    native Spanish bilingual students” (p. 510). As a result, a positive correlation was

    determined between vocabulary and English spellings through the application of the

    lexical restructuring model. In contrast, a study conducted by Wang and Geva (2003)

    reported that despite having poorer English vocabulary scores, Chinese ESL children’s

    overall spelling performance compared to L1 children was of no difference. Keity and

    Harrison (2015) reported similar results, where L1 and ESL children little differences in

    their spelling sophistication scores despite ESL children having lower oral vocabulary

    and syntactic knowledge.

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    22/65

      21

    !"5"@ A+*94'B1' *C D*&()*4*1<

    Morphological information is described as the “component parts of words (morphemes)

    and the way in which words are reconstructed as meaning changes” (Westwood, 2014, p.

    17). It should be noted that morphemes are “the smallest meaningful part of a word andcan be a prefix, suffix, plurals” (Ott, 2007, p. 287). Having morphological awareness

    involves being able to recognize the root words, compound words, prefixes, suffixes and

    later on the origin and derivations of words (Westwood, 2014). Notably, Carlisle (1995)

    has stated, “the extent of a child’s morphological awareness is limited by his/her

    morphological knowledge” (p. 194). Children are at the final stages of mastering the

    inflections when they begin to read and write at the kindergarten and grade one level

    (Berko, 1958; Brown, 1973; Clay, 1982; de Villiers & de Villiers, 1973 as cited in

    Carlisle, 1995).

    Treiman, and Cassar (1996) depicted that children did not use morphological relations

    among words even though they could have. Nevertheless, the study showed that when

    children are spelling, they are capable of using other sources of information to spell,

    which includes certain morphological relationships among words (Treiman & Cassar,

    1996). Furthermore, they stipulated that young children are capable of using

    morphological strategies only when there are a few demands competing for their attention

    (Treiman & Cassar, 1996). Jones (1991 as cited in Carlisle, 1995) reported similar results,

    where young children showed “awareness to the relation of surface representations and

    underlying forms of morphologically complex words” (p. 195). Thereby demonstrating

    the possibility that morphological awareness may be involved in spelling at an earlier

    stage in invented spelling.

    ;GD I($(3:,- %3 :$>($,() -6('':$*

    Vacca et al. (2009) stated that the gradual sophistication of children’s invented spellings

    should be celebrated as a display of intelligence and emergent competence in the written

    language. Spelling errors made by children should not be seen as random nor as being out

    of ignorance instead it should be seen as genuine attempts to apply the alphabetic

     principle to written English based on their knowledge of phonics and sound-symbol

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    23/65

      22

    relationships (Savage, 2011). According to Kolodziej and Columba (2005), educators

    who informed parents of the positive benefits in regards to invented spelling were more

    likely to change their attitudes and become more accepting.

    !"@"# E8(&*7'/ .*+7'+,-*+04 /('44-+1

    Spelling is a developmental process that children progress through as they develop their

    level of sophistication and cognitive awareness on how spelling works. Gentry and Gillet

    (1993) have stated that while invented spelling ultimately leads to conventional spelling,

    it is not merely about acquiring expert spelling ability but setting the foundation for

    expert spelling to be constructed from. The phonics element of invented spelling allows

    children to interact and better understand how the alphabetic principle works and

    involves them in meaningful writing. Therefore, the processes involved in invented

    spelling allow children to actively construct and internalize the rules of spelling, which in

    turn helps them progress to conventional spelling (Kolodziej and Columba, 2005).

    !"@"! E8(&*7'/ &'0B-+1 /;-44

    Invented spelling provides children with excellent and valuable practice for them to apply

    their understanding of “phonetics, word analysis and synthesis, and letter-sound

    correspondences” (Chomsky, 1979, p. 48). The exploratory nature of invented spelling

     provides the opportunity for children to use an analytical approach, which promotes the

    integration of the phonological and alphabetic information into initial lexical

    representations thereby connecting the phonological and orthographic information

    (Ouellette & Sénéchal, 2008). In turn these lexical representations, once refined, may

     possibly facilitate the acquisition of reading.

    When young children write, they are applying already internalized grapheme-phoneme

     principles in order to spell words they have not yet mastered the conventional spellings of

    (Clarke, 1987; Paul, 1976; Richgels, 2002 as cited in He & Wang, 2009). Children are

     provided with a natural context, in which they have an authentic involvement with

     phonics as they attempt to spell their desired word. They are engaged in applying their

    “skills to segment phonemes and to represent them with corresponding graphemes” (He

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    24/65

      23

    & Wang, 2009, p. 46). As a result, young children writers are able to “enhance their

     phonological awareness, refine their phoneme segmentation skills, and establish as well

    as modify their grapheme-phoneme principles” (He & Wang, 2009, p. 46). Furthermore,

     phonological knowledge required in invented spelling strongly correlates to beginning

    readers’ success (Burns & Richgels, 1989; Winsor, 1990).

    !"@"2 E8(&*7'/ 9&-,-+1 /;-44

    Invented spelling provides children with the opportunity to be able to write freely, and

    experience a sense of control over the written word. While conventional spelling creates a

     barrier, and restricts them as they become afraid to make educated guesses based on their

    own understanding of the orthographic correspondences (Savage, 2011). While children

    have large oral vocabularies, it should be noted that their spelling vocabularies are small

    in comparison (Cramer, 1998). Through invented spelling, children are able to interact

    with written language and develop understanding on the relationship between the written

    word and their thoughts.

    Several research studies have indicated that when young children focused more on the

    message of their writing as opposed to their spelling, their writing achievement was

     positively affected (Gettinger, 1993; Nicholson, 1996). Bear et al. (2004) have stated that

    a hesitant writer, who focuses on conventional spelling, will lose the chance to express

    their new ideas, and invented spelling allows them to freely write down their ideas.

    Furthermore, Healy (1991) who found that journal writing of children using invented

    spellings were three times longer and included more detail, elaboration and imaginative

    language compared to children who relied on traditional spelling. In addition, young

    children who used invented spellings were found to write longer and more fluent pieces

    of writing compared to those who did not (Paul, 1976).

    ;GE 25( "1-( %3 45:$(-( LK< '(10$(0- 1$) -6('':$*M :$>($,() -6('':$*

    Bilingual speakers may use the literacy skills developed in one language when reading

    and spelling in another language (Marinova-Todd & Hall, 2013). There has been

    evidence on the transfer of phonological awareness skills across other languages (Perfetti

    & Liu, 2005; Gottardo et. al, 2001). Rickard Liow and Poon (1998) support this by

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    25/65

      24

    concluding there is a possibility of the L1 influencing the nature and development of

     phonological awareness in the second language, which could potentially affect the

    strategies applied by children when reading and spelling in their second language.

    A study conducted by He and Wang (2009) explored ESL beginner writers and their use

    of invented spellings, and learned that even at a very young age, they were already aware

    of the grapheme-phoneme principles needed in spelling English words. They were fully

    capable of representing phonemes they either segmented or pronounced with the

    corresponding graphemes accordingly. On the one hand, the young ESL writers with their

    invented spellings show that they may have inaccurately or inappropriately distinguished

    articulated phonemes (He & Wang, 2009). On the other hand, it shows they are able to

    apply phonologically sophisticated principles in a systematic manner (He & Wang, 2009).

    Hanley et al. (1999) have pointed out that children employed a whole-word, visual, and

    rote learning strategy when they are taught to read and write in Chinese. It should be

    noted that different teaching approaches in reading Chinese influence phonological

     processing skills (Wang and Geva, 2003), and Chinese readers who used an alphabetic

     phonetic system such as Pinyin were more successful at manipulating speech sounds than

    those who became literate through Chinese characters (Read, Zhang, Nie & Ding, 1986

    as cited in Wang & Geva, 2003). This seems to imply that having exposure to an

    alphabetic phonetic system regardless of whether it was Pinyin or English that it would

     positively influence their phonological processing skills. It was found that there was a

     positive transfer effect from the L1’s reading experience, where they seemed to better use

    their visual orthographic memory of a letter string “ensemble” compared to Chinese ESL

    learners (Wang & Geva, 2003).

    Similar results were reported by Dixon et al. (2010), where the visual approach benefited

    Chinese ESL learners on the conventional spelling of high-frequency words in English.

    Considering that Malay is also an alphabetic language and possesses a shallow

    orthography, Chinese ESL learners were able to perform better than them on the

    conventional spelling of English high frequency words. However they argued that the

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    26/65

      25

     poorer phonological processing skills observed in Chinese ESL learners could possibly

    lead to future difficulty in spelling unfamiliar words in English.

    Wade-Woolley and Siegel (1997) found no difference between L1 and Chinese ESL

    learners on a real word and pseudoword spelling task even though Chinese ESL learners

     performed poorly on the phonological tasks. While, Marinova-Todd and Hall (2007)

    reported Chinese ESL learners as having higher scores on real-world spelling tasks. In

    addition, they were portrayed to have the fewest associations between phonological

    awareness and spelling, which was suggested to have occurred due to their reliance on

    other processing skills besides phonological awareness when spelling pseudowords.

    Similarly, Wang and Geva (2003) found that Chinese ESL learners performed better on

    spelling familiar real words than on unknown pseudowords. A possible explanation could

     be that as Chinese ESL learners are spelling familiar or real words they are able to rely on

    the direct and nonphonological route, which is what has already been acquired and

     practiced in their L1 reading and writing activities (Holm & Dodd, 1996 as cited in Wang

    & Geva, 2003).

    ;GN O5%$%'%*:"1' ):33(0($"(- /(,&(($ 41$,%$(-( 1$) L$*':-5

    Cantonese and English are two languages that are typologically distant. They differ in

    many respects, which include their phoneme inventories, characteristics of sounds,

    distribution of phonemes, permissible syllable structure and rhythm (Chan & Li, 2000).

    This poses a degree of difficulty for Cantonese speakers when mastering the standard

    English pronunciation. It has been suggested in previous research findings that a learner’s

     previous linguistic experience would influence their learning of other languages (Corder,

    1883; Hung, 1993 as cited in Chan & Li, 2000). Therefore the English pronunciation

     problems made by native speakers of Cantonese could be attributed to the phonological

    differences between the two respective languages of English, and Cantonese (Chan & Li,

    2000).

    Taking into consideration on how invented spelling represents a child’s attempt to create

    versions of words through utilizing their current understanding of letter-sound

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    27/65

      26

    relationships, they are spelling a word based on the way it sounds to them. In order for

    children to be able to spell, they need to be able to orally segment the word into

     phonemes, map the individual sounds to their corresponding letter, write letters in the

    correct order and then read the word to make sure it is spelled correctly (Savage, 2011).

    Therefore a child’s pronunciation of the word itself plays a huge part in how they

    ultimately decide to spell a word. It is important to be aware of the phonological

    differences as they can affect how a Chinese ESL learner determines how to spell a word.

    Chan and Li (2000) outline and discuss the pronunciation problems and difficulties faced

     by Chinese ESL learners. The pronunciation problems were divided into consonants,

    vowels, diphthongs and rhythm. All these categories with exception to rhythm will be

     briefly discussed as they could possibly account for the invented spelling errors made by

    Chinese ESL learners.

    Plosives

    Both languages have six plosive stops which include bilabial plosives /p, b/, alveolar

     plosives /t, d/ and velar plosives /k, g/, however, in Cantonese these are all voiceless. In

    comparison, /p, t, k/ are voiceless while /b, d, g/ are voiced. When the sound is voiced,

    this refers to how the vocal chords vibrate when the sound is articulated (Savage, 2011).

    In contrast, voiceless refers to when the vocal chords do not vibrate as the sound is made

    (Savage, 2011). In Cantonese, there are no voiced syllable-final plosives, which results in

    the substitution of the phonemes /p, t. k/ for /b, d, g/ (Chan & Li, 2000).

    Fricatives

    Fricatives refer to articulations that “involve turbulent airflow through a narrow channel,

    resulting in a hissing noise” (Catford, 1994, p. 17). There are nine fricatives in English,

    which include the labio-dental /f, v/, dental /!, ! " /, alveolar /s, z/, post-alveolar / ! , "/, and

    glottal /h/ (Chan & Li, 2000). The following sounds among the English fricatives, which

    are voiced include /v, ! " , z, "/ and the rest /f, #, s,  ! , h/ are voiceless (Chan & Li, 2000).

    While in Cantonese, there are only three fricatives, which are the labio-dental /f/, alveolar

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    28/65

      27

    /s/, and glottal /h/ (Chan & Li, 2000). All of which are voiceless but they are articulated

    similarly to English.

    Chinese ESL learners tend to substitute a sound that is close to their native language

    when coming across a difficult or unfamiliar sound. This is evident as few of the

    fricatives are substituted with another sound since few of these fricatives /z, !, ! " , "/ do

    not exist in Cantonese. The voiced labio-dental fricative sound /v/, at the beginning of the

    word the sound /w/ is substituted for /v/, and at the end of a word /f/ for /v/ (Chan & Li,

    2000). While, the voiced alveolar fricative phoneme /z/ is replaced with its voiceless

    counterpart /s/ (Chan & Li, 2000). The dental fricatives /!, ! " / are often substituted with

    either /t/ or /f/ and /d/ or /f/ respectively (Chan & Li, 2000). The “th” sounds represented

     by /!, ! " /. The voiceless palato-alveolar fricative / ! / is often substituted with /s/ (Chan &

    Li, 2000). Lastly, the voiced palato-alveolar fricative /"/ is substituted with /s/ or / ! /.

    Affricates

    Affricates are a “stop released into homorganic fricative” (Cartford, 1994, p. 220).

    Chinese ESL learners tend to have a problem with two particular English affricates,

    which are /t ! / and /d"/ (Chan & Li, 2000). These two English affricates /t ! / and /d"/ are

    substituted with their Chinese counterparts /ts/ and /dz/, respectively (Chan & Li, 2000).

     Nasals, laterals and / $/

    Chinese ESL learners have problems in distinguishing the sound /l/ from /n/ or / $/, where

    the / $/ at the beginning of words such as ‘ride’ / $a%d/ and ‘road’ / $&'d/ is substituted with

    an /l/ -like sound (Chan & Li, 2000). Also, some speakers may consequently substitute

    /w/ for / $/. In addition, there is variation when /l/ and /n/ are in the syllable-initial position.

    At times, /l/ may be substituted for /n/ at the beginning of words and sometimes the initial

    /l/ may be pronounced with an “n” quality, which gives a nasalized /l/ sound (Chan & Li,

    2000).

    Pure vowels

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    29/65

      28

    Often the phonemes /æ/ and /e/ are indistinguishable to Chinese ESL learners and usually

    the phoneme /e/ is substituted for /æ/ (Chan & Li, 2000). They also have problems with

    long and short vowel pairs, where they are unable to tell them apart, which includes /i:/

    and /%/, /u:/ and /'/, and /(:/ and / )/ (Chan & Li, 2000). The errors that the Chinese ESL

    learners make are unique to the individual, at times, they may use a short vowel sound to

    represent a long vowel sound, and vice versa.

    Diphthongs

    Diphthongs are often replaced with a pure short vowel that sounds similar to the Chinese

    ESL learners as they have difficulty in articulating certain diphthongs (Chan & Li, 2000).

    An example of which would be with the word ‘point’ /p(%nt/ may be pronounced as /p )nt/

    where /(%/ is replaced with the short vowel / )/ (Chan & Li, 2000). Similarly with the

    diphthong /e%/ is replaced with the pure vowel /e/ (Chan & Li, 2000).

    Consonant Clusters

    Since there are no consonant clusters in Cantonese, Chinese ESL learners tend to cope

    with this difference by using either deletion or epenthesis, where deletion is when the

    consonant cluster is reduced and epenthesis is when the consonant cluster is broken up

    through the addition of a vowel thereby creating an extra syllable and the onset contains

    only one consonant (Chan & Li, 2000).

    Word-final consonant clusters

    When Chinese ESL learners encounter word-final consonant clusters, they tend to delete

    one or more of the consonants, which are often the phonemes /l, t, d/. In addition,

    Chinese ESL learners tend to have problems with pronouncing words in the past tense as

    such the past tense marker sound /t/ or /d/ is dropped (Chan & Li, 2000). Epenthesis may

    also be used when facing word-final consonant clusters, where an extra syllable is added

    so ‘film’ /f %lm/ becomes /f %l&m/ (Chan & Li, 2000).

    Word-initial consonant clusters

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    30/65

      29

    Word-initial consonant clusters are often simplified through either deletion or epenthesis,

    and an example would be when / $ / is deleted after a plosive, where ‘produce’ /p $&dju:s/

     becomes /p&dju:s/ (Chan & Li, 2000). Another example would be an epenthesis, where an

    extra syllable is added with ‘clutch’ /kl*t ! / becoming /kal*t ! /.

    A study conducted by Wang and Geva (2003) compared the lexical and visual-

    orthographic processing in the spelling performance of Chinese ESL learners and native-

    English speaking children. In the pseudoword spelling to dictation task conducted, they

    used the consonant digraphs (sh, th) and consonant clusters (st, sp) because it is absent in

    the L1 phonology of the Chinese ESL learners. They discovered the presence of negative

    transfer attributed to the absence of certain phonological units such as th and sh was

    evident when they were in grade 1 but not in grade 2, which was when they took part in

    the research. Therefore, it was concluded that the L1-specific transfer only occurs in the

    early stages of learning their second language, and it did not affect their spelling

     performance.

    After reviewing the literature, there is limited research specifically looking at the

    invented spellings made by Chinese ESL learners in the first grade. This poses to be an

    area of interest as there appears to be a significant growth in Chinese ESL learner’s

    spelling development between the first and second grade as described by Wang and Geva

    (2003). It would be beneficial to track and examine their invented spellings made

    throughout the year to understand Chinese ESL learner’s spelling development progress,

    and determine how to address their learning needs accordingly. This resulted in the

    following research questions, which seek to explore the invented spellings made by grade

    one children:

    =(-(10"5 P9(-,:%$-1.  What are the common errors found in grade one children’s invented spelling?

    2.  What are the similarities and differences in the common errors in invented

    spelling made by grade one children between the first and second term?

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    31/65

      30

    4516,(0 ?F =(-(10"5 J(-:*$ 1$) @(,5%)%'%*A

    The research questions outlined in this chapter served the purpose of narrowing down the

    scope of research. The research methods chosen for the use of this research will be

    explained with supported literature. Detailed information on the nature of the researchand methodology will be given by providing information on the research site and

     participants. The data collection and data analysis will also be included and explained in

    detail.

    ?G7 =(-(10"5 P9(-,:%$-

    1.  What are the common errors found in grade one children’s invented spelling?

    2.  What are the similarities and differences in the common errors in invented

    spelling made by grade one children between the first and second term?

    ?G; @(,5%)%'%*:"1' !660%1"5

    The methodological approach implemented for this research was an intrinsic case study,

    which are defined as studies that attempt to understand the particular case in question

    (Stake, 1994). The particular case, in this research, are the invented spellings made by a

    single grade one class. The research aims to examine and understand the invented

    spellings in attempts to develop a better understanding on the types of spelling errors

    made by Chinese ESL learners.

    ?G? =(-(10"5 -(,,:$*

    The school is an English Medium of Instruction (EMI) primary school in Shek Mun,

    Hong Kong. The school has a school-based curriculum, and draws reference from a

    Western curriculum. There is a whole school approach towards invented spelling, and

    this is implemented and encouraged by all English teachers in the school.

    ?GB O10,:":61$,-

    The research sample was of convenience sampling, which comprised of the collected

    writing summative assessments from a single grade one class. From the time of the first

    writing summative assessment, the children were aged from 5-6 years old. All of the

    children are ESL learners and have Cantonese as their L1. The grade one class consisted

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    32/65

      31

    of twenty-two children, who have been streamed into the class based on their English

    ability. There were sixteen girls and six boys. These children were streamed into the

    middle group therefore showing average English ability. Each child has a total of four

    writing summative assessments they have completed over one academic school year.

    After obtaining ethical consent, the sampling was narrowed down to twenty children. The

    sample consists of writing summative assessments from fifteen girls and five boys.

    ?GD O0%"()90(

    Children’s writing summative assessments, which they have completed over the

    academic year 2014-2015, were collected. These were collected in order to identify their

    invented spellings. Within one school term, there were two writing summative

    assessments. The academic year consisted of two school terms, which equates to fourwriting summative assessments given and completed to students.

    The writing summative assessments were based on an unknown topic given to the

    children, which they were then given space to write about. However, the writing genre

    and assessment criteria remained the same as the writing formative assessment completed

    in class. All children were given the same topic respective of the term. They were given

    forty-five minutes to complete the assessment. The writing summative assessments

    referred to those that were completed during these time periods as shown:

    Table 1: Writing summative assessment timeline

    Summative assessment Period

    Term 1 Mid-term Writing Assessment October 2014

    Term 1 End-term Writing Assessment December 2014

    Term 2 Mid-term Writing Assessment March 2015

    Term 2 End-term Writing Assessment June 2015

    They were not given any help with their spelling during the writing summative

    assessment. Therefore, the invented spellings reflect their own attempts at conventional

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    33/65

      32

    spelling. The writing summative assessments were collected after marking, grading and

    data entry had been completed. In total, there were eighty samples collected from their

    writing summative assessment across the academic school year.

    ?GE L,5:"1' "%$-:)(01,:%$-

    Before distributing the consent forms to all the participants (i.e. children’s guardian and

    school principal), the ethical requirements of the university were met accordingly.

    Participants’ confidentiality was ensured as they were aware of their right to review or

    withdraw the data they submitted, as well as how the data collected would be kept

    confidential and used for the research purpose.

    Participants were required to give informed consent and provided detailed information onthe researcher’s intent and purpose in conducting the research before data collection

    commenced. As stated on the consent form, they had the right to withdraw at any time

    during the data collection period.

    ?G N J1,1 1$1'A-:- +(,5%)

    After the collection of the data, the writing summative assessments were each examined.

    Any misspellings of words were considered as a form of invented spelling and recorded

    onto a spreadsheet. The invented spellings were grouped according to when they were

    completed. The conventional spelling of the word was determined by the context in

    which the invented spelling was written in. Afterwards, it was written next to the

    invented spelling.

    2"F"# G('44-+1 /*()-/,-.0,-*+H G.*&-+1 8',)*B

    The scoring method chosen to assess children’s spelling sophistication is an adapted

    version of Treiman and Bourassa’s (2000) (See Appendix 1) scoring method. Originally,

    Treiman and Bourassa’s (2000) scoring method is a composite system, which is able to

    examine the phonological and orthographic features of children’s spellings. The adapted

    version taken from Yeong and Rickard Liow (2011) was chosen to evaluate the invented

    spellings made by children because it was perceptive to the spelling developmental

    sequences observed. Yeong and Rickard Liow (2011) used this scoring system in a

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    34/65

      33

    similar context with ethnic Chinese children, aged 5 years old and English as their second

    language. This particular scoring method took into consideration of the difficulty

    highlighted by Treiman and Bourassa (2000), which occurred when children grouped

    separated phonemes together. The number of points was increased in order to account for

    another level of sophistication reached when a child is able to represent all four sounds in

    a word. This scoring method was used to track and provide a deeper understanding on the

    children’s spelling progress across the academic school year.

    The scoring system was adapted (Table 1) so that it became a 10-point scale (0-9). The

    last and highest score of 10 was omitted because it represented that conventional spelling

    was produced. However, the purpose of the research was to collect and examine all of the

    invented spellings. The scoring system was able to assess the phonological and

    orthographic features present in children’s spellings since the number of points increased

    when children were able to represent more sounds found in the target word in their

    spelling. This helped determine any patterns in their invented spellings and these were

    grouped according to the six developmental stages of spelling, which was described

    earlier.

    Interrater reliability was assessed by having two raters score all of the invented spellings

    recorded. Another English teacher was invited to use both scoring methods to score all of

    the invented spellings. When a disagreement arose in the scoring, the invented spelling

    was reviewed by the raters and a consensus score was given.

    Table 2: Spelling sophistication scoring method for hypothetical spelling of “train”

    Spelling Description Points

    7 Does not include any letters 0

    R2D Includes some letters that are not related to the sounds in the word

    and some other symbols

    1

    ohreas, pl Includes only letters that are not related to sounds in the word 2

    risv, Begins with letter that is related in sound to initial phonemic or with 3

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    35/65

      34

    nesenn a letter that represents a phoneme other than initial phoneme

    t, tiso Begins with a conventional initial letter 4

    tad, tissn Two of four phonemes represented with conventional letters, letters

    related in sound or highly visually confusable with conventional

    letters. Intrusions allowed

    5

    tadre,

    triap

    Three of four phonemes represented with conventional letters,

    letters related in sound or highly visually confusable with

    conventional letters. Intrusions allowed.

    6

    tain, taen Three of four phonemes represented with initial and final consonant

    represented conventionally. Intrusions allowed.

    7

    tarin,

    triatn

    All four phonemes represented with initial and final consonant

    represented conventionally. Intrusion allowed.

    8

    tran, tren All four phonemes represented with all consonants represented

    conventionally. No intrusions allowed.

    9

     Note  Adapted from Yeong, S. H. M. & Rickard Liow, S. J. (2011). Cognitive-linguistic foundations of

    early spelling development in bilinguals. Journal of Educational Psychology, p. 7, Table 1.

    2"F"! 60,'1*&-I0,-*+ *C -+7'+,'B /('44-+1/

    After the invented spellings were grouped, each of the groupings was looked at in detail

    and the errors were listed accordingly. The errors were categorized into four main

    categories of substitutions, omissions, insertions and transpositions. These categories

    were taken from a research conducted by Cook (1997), which explored the spelling errors

    made by second language speakers of English. By drawing from spelling errors examined

    in previous works, the four main categories were used as a skeleton in order to provide a

    more detailed description of typical errors made by L1 and L2 English language speakers.

    This is applicable to the current research as it is able to provide a foundation on which to

     base the spelling errors made the Chinese ESL learners. This provided greater insight as

    to the specific errors the children were making across the school year based on their

    writing summative assessments.

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    36/65

      35

    4516,(0 BF C:$):$*-

    In this chapter, the data collected from the children’s writing summative assessments will

     be presented. The invented spellings of children will be sorted according to their level of

    error. Based on their respective terms, the spelling errors made will then be categorizedinto four main categories. The findings will be presented in a manner to answer the two

    questions, which are (1) What are the common errors found in grade one children’s

    invented spelling? (2) What are the similarities and differences in the common errors in

    invented spelling made by grade one children between the first and second term?

    BG7 25( $9+/(0 %3 :$>($,() -6('':$*- 1"0%-- ,5( ,&% ,(0+-

    From children’s writing summative assessments, a total of 548 words were considered to

     be invented spellings as they were seen as children’s genuine attempts in spelling a word.

    Figure 1 presents the total number of invented spellings identified from each of the

    writing summative assessments. The percentage of invented spellings was calculated

    using the equation (a/548) x 100, where a represented the number of invented spellings

    identified from the writing summative assessments.

    Most of the invented spellings identified were taken from the first mid-term writing

    summative assessment, which had a percentage of 33.76%. This could be attributed tochildren having just started their formal instruction in English and as such have not had

    as much exposure to the English language. Then, the second end-term writing summative

    assessments accounted for 26.09% of invented spellings identified. From the first end-

    term and second mid-term writing summative assessment, a similar number of invented

    spellings were identified 20.44& and 19.71% respectively. Overall, a total of 54.19% of

    invented spellings were identified from the first term writing summative assessment, and

    the remaining 45.81% from the second term writing summative assessment.

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    37/65

      36

    Figure 1: The invented spellings across the two terms 

    BG; K6('':$* -%65:-,:"1,:%$ :$ ,5( 3:0-, ,(0+

    Based on the spelling sophistication scoring method, the invented spellings were grouped

    according to their respective scores. The scores each represented a specific error feature

    that had to be identified in the invented spelling. Figure 2 shows the score points given

    from the 10-point scale (0-9) and the percentage of invented spellings that were given the

     particular score. From the first term writing summative assessments, a total of 297

    invented spellings were identified.

    A total of 185 words were identified as invented spellings from the first mid-term writing

    summative assessment. A majority of the invented spellings were given a score point of 6

    or 7, and shared the same percentage of invented spellings (23.24%). Then, the invented

    spellings scoring an 8 followed after with 16.22%. The score point with the least

     percentage of invented spellings was 2 (2.16%). This represented the least error of this

    type in the first mid-term.

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    38/65

      37

    Figure 2: The invented spellings in the first mid-term

    Figure 3 shows the invented spellings in the first end-term. In comparison, there was

    considerably less number of words identified as invented spellings in the first end-term

    writing summative assessments. Only 112 words were identified as being invented

    spellings. Noticeably, there was an increase in the percentage of invented spellings being

    scored a 9 (22.32%). Similar to the first mid-term assessment, the percentage of invented

    spellings scoring a 6 or 7 was fairly similar with 22.32% and 24.11% respectively.

    Interestingly, the score point of 4 had the least percentage of invented spellings (0.89%).

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    39/65

      38

    Figure 3: The invented spellings in the first end-term

    Figure 4 combines the results from the first mid-term and end-term writing summative

    assessment. This provided a general assessment on the invented spelling made by

    children in the first term. None of the invented spellings received a score point of 100 or

    1. The score point of 2 had the least percentage of invented spellings (1.35%). The

    majority of the invented spellings seemed to fall under the score point of 6 (22.3%) and 7

    (23.57%).

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    40/65

      39

    Figure 4: All of the invented spellings from the first term

    BG? K6('':$* -%65:-,:"1,:%$ :$ ,5( -("%$) ,(0+

    From the second term writing summative assessments, a total of 251 words were

    identified as invented spelling. Figure 5 shows the invented spellings made by children in

    their second mid-term writing summative assessment. In the second mid-term writing

    summative assessment, 108 words were identified as invented spelling. A majority of the

    invented spellings have been scored either an 8 (22.22%) or 9 (27.8%). Similar to the first

    term, the score point of 6 and 7 share similar percentages of invented spellings, which are

    8.52% and 17.59%, respectively. The score points of 2 (1.85%), 3 (1.85%) and 4 (0.93%)

    had the least percentage of invented spellings.

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    41/65

      40

    Figure 5: The invented spellings in the second mid-term

    Figure 6 shows the invented spellings identified from the second end-term writing

    summative assessment. A total of 143 words were identified as invented spelling. The

    results differed from the second mid-term writing summative assessment in that the score

     point of 7 had the highest percentage of invented spelling (24.4%). This was closely

    followed by score points 8 (23.78%) and 9 (22.38%). There were no invented spelling

    errors made between the score points 0 to 2. The second end-term writing summative

    assessment had a higher percentage of invented spellings receiving a score point of 7

    compared to the second mid-term writing summative assessment. There was also a

    significant decrease in the percentage of invented spellings made under the score point 9.

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    42/65

      41

    Figure 6: The invented spellings in the second end-term

    Figure 7 combines the invented spelling errors made in the second mid-term and end-

    term writing summative assessment. Generally, there appears to be a descending

     percentage of invented spelling going down the score points. The score point 9 has the

    highest percentage of invented spellings (24.7%), which is then followed by the score

     point 8 (23.11%) and 7 (23.57%). Interestingly, the score point of 3 (2.4%) has the higher

     percentage of invented spellings compared to the score point of 4 (1.2%). The lowest

     percentage of invented spellings still remains to the score point of 2 with 0.8%.

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    43/65

      42

    Figure 7: All of the invented spellings from the second term

    BGB 4%+610:-%$ %3 -6('':$* -%65:-,:"1,:%$ :$ ,5( 3:0-, 1$) -("%$) ,(0+

    Figure 8 compares the invented spellings, using the scoring method, across the first and

    second term. The first and second term show a similar pattern in the distribution of

    invented spelling errors made across the 10-point scale. Both terms show children’s

    invented spellings being scored from 2 to 7 with a slight difference. There is a smaller

     percentage of invented spelling errors made in the second term. Interestingly, in the first

    term the score point 8 and 9 have a lower percentage of invented spelling in comparison

    to the second term.

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    44/65

      43

    Figure 8: The invented spellings across the two terms

    BGD 8$>($,() -6('':$* (00%0- +1)( :$ ,5( 3:0-, ,(0+

    From the first mid-term writing summative assessment, only 2.16% of invented spellings

    were given a score point of 2, which indicates that a small portion of the words include

    letters that are not related to the words. This means that the remaining 97.84% are able to

     begin their words with a letter that is related in sound to the initial phoneme in the word

    they want to spell. Most of the invented spellings are related in sound to a phoneme in

    their respective word. The words are often spelled with the correct conventional letter at

    the beginning of the word. However, there are a few that are not spelled with the

    conventional letter at the beginning. These words have spelling errors that are related to

    substitution, where one letter is replaced with another letter. A common error was with

    the /+/ sound, where it was being substituted with the letter /f/.

    The rest of the spelling errors encountered were related to the middle and end sounds.

    This was often due to vowel substitutions made by the child. The vowel sounds were

    often represented with a vowel that had the same sounding letter name. In addition,

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    45/65

      44

    consonant substitutions affected the word ending as children substituted similar sounding

    vowels or consonants in place of the correct one. This resulted in words such as pleys,

    where the /s/ is represented with the letter s itself.

    Based on the score points, 50.27% of invented spellings received a score lower than 7.

    This indicates the percentage of invented spellings that have zero to three phonemes

    represented. As such, there was a large portion of invented spellings that had omissions.

    The invented spellings were either missing the silent vowel, a consonant cluster, vowel,

    syllable or showed consonant doubling. Among the omissions, the omission of a vowel

    was the most common spelling error made. In comparison, there were less insertion and

    transposition spelling errors made by children at this stage.

    Table 3: Invented spelling errors made in the first mid-term writing summative assessment

    Spelling error Examples of invented spellings

    Substitution

    Vowel substitution

    -Representing the sound of /u:/ with /'/

    -Representing the sound of /i/ with the letter e

    -Approximating the sound of /(/ by representing it

    with the letter o

    -Approximating the sound of /,/ with the letter a

     because its name /e/ is similar to /,/

    -Approximating the sound of /%/ with the letter e

     because its name /i/ is similar to /%/

    -Representing the sound /,/ with the letter e because

    its sound is /e/ 

     Fud (food) 

     Mumne (money), tide (tidy), mane (many), candee

    (candy)

    Owst (always)

     Dask (desk), pancil (pencil), bast (best)

     Eeve (if)

     Frens (friend)

    Consonant substitution

    -Representing the sound /&n/ with the letter n

    -Approximating the sound of /l/ with the letter o 

     because /o/ is similar to /l/ 

    -Representing the sound /s/ with the letter  s because

    the sound is /s/

    Crdrn (children)

    Tabo (table), pipo (people), aoso (also)

     Pleys (place), introdus (introduce), bekos (because),

    nis (nice)

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    46/65

      45

    -Representing the sound /+/ with the letter f

    -Representing the sound of /w/ with the letter r

    -Approximating the sound of /t/ and /k/ with the

    letter d  

     Fing (thing), fink (think)

     Ritboard (whiteboard), rad  (wide)

     Ardwerd (artwork)

    Omission-Omission of silent vowel

    -Consonant cluster reduction

    -Consonant cluster deletion

    -Omission of a vowel

    -Omission of a syllable

    -Consonant doubling

    Thy (they), Mak (make)

     Libaryion (librarian)

    Sool (school)

    Corr (classroom), bar  (borrow)

     Her (hear)

     Rubish (Rubbish), runing (running)

    Insertion Gentaly (gently), colock (clock)

    Transposition Calss (class) 

    Table 3 shows the invented spelling errors made by children in the first end-term writing

    summative assessment. In comparison, there is a lower percentage of 38.39% of invented

    spellings that have zero to three phonemes represented. This indicates that the remaining

    invented spellings had more phonemes represented, which is shown by how 95.54% are

    spelled with the correct conventional initial letter. The spelling errors were mainly related

    to the middle and end sound of the words. A minimum of two to three phonemes was

    represented in every word. The common error type was the omission of vowels, and

    consonant clusters.

    Table 4: Invented spelling errors made in the first end-term writing summative assessment

    Spelling error Examples of invented spellings

    Substitution

    Vowel substitution

    - The /i/ sound represented with the letter e

    -The sound of /(/ approximated by representing it

    with the letter o

    -The sound of /i/ represented by the letter e

    -The sound of /,/ approximated with the letter a

     because its name /e/ is similar to /,/

    -The sound of /aI/ represented by the letter I  

     yumme (yummy)

    Osle (also), caso (castle)

    Cooke (cookie)

     Bad (bed)

    Tim (time), rid (ride)

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    47/65

      46

    -The sound of /e/ represented by the letter a Classmat (classmate)

    Consonant substitution

    -The sound of /l/ approximated with the letter o 

     because /o/ is similar to /l/ 

    -The sound /+/ represented with the letter d-The sound /+/ represented with the letter f

    -The sound /t ! / represented by the letter string ch

     because its sound is /t ! / 

    -The sound of /z/ approximated with the letter s

    -The sound of /f/ represented with the letter v

    -The sound /p/ approximated with the letter b

     Aso (also), caso (castle)

     Dat (that)wife (with)

     pecher (picture)

     Desey (dizzy)

     Haf (have)

     Pinseboll (Principal)

    Omission

    -Omission of silent vowel

    -Consonant cluster reduction

    -Consonant cluster deletion

    -Omission of a vowel

    -Consonant doubling

    mak (make)

     playgane (playground), wen (when)

    mon (month)

    tim (tim), tek (take), scoter (scooter)

    allmost (almost), siting (sitting), canot (cannot)

    Insertion  Morening (morning)

    Transposition  Paly (play), friut (fruit)

    BGE 8$>($,() -6('':$* (00%0- +1)( :$ ,5( -("%$) ,(0+

    Comparatively, the second mid-term writing summative assessment shows children’s

    development towards more sophisticated spelling. Only 32.41% make invented spellings

    that have a score point of 6. This demonstrates that children are even more capable at

    representing more of the phonemes they hear in a word. This has led to a noticeable

     pattern in their spellings where there are more invented spellings with insertions as

    children try to represent all of the phonemes. Children are becoming more capable in

    representing the beginning and end sound of the word.

    Table 5: Invented spelling errors made in the second mid-term writing summative assessment

    Spelling error Examples of invented spellings

    Substitution

    Vowel substitution

    -The sound of /i/ represented by the letter e  Pelole (pillow), slipe (sleepy)

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    48/65

      47

    -The sound of /,/ approximated with the letter a

     because its name /e/ is similar to /,/

    -The sound of /*/ approximated by representing it

    with the letter a

     Badroom (bedroom), tham (them)

     Brash (brush)

    Consonant substitution

    -The sound of /+/ represented with the letter d

    -The sound of /+/ represented with the letter f

    -The sound /b/ approximated with the letter p 

    -The sound /d/ approximated with the letter p

    -The sound /d/ approximated with the letter t

     De (the), ader (other), den (then)

    Theef (teeth), clofs (clothes)

     Planket (blanket),

     Hoop (hood)

     Nit (need), net (need)

    Omission

    -Omission of silent vowel

    -Consonant cluster reduction

    -Omission of a vowel

    -Consonant doubling

     Pople (people)

     Bush (brush), seep (sleep)

     stry (story), teth (teeth)

    brrush (brush), of (off)

    Insertion  finieshes (finish), myslelf (myself) 

    Transposition Siad (said), saop (soap)

    In the second end-term writing summative assessment, there is a decrease in the

     percentage of invented spellings (29.37%), which has a score less than 6. This means that

    a higher percentage of invented spellings are spelled with two or more phonemesrepresented. They were able to spell their words using the correct and appropriate basic

    conventional letter. There is an increase in invented spellings that are spelled with

    insertions present compared to previous writing summative assessments.

    Table 6: Invented spelling errors made in the second end-term writing summative assessment

    Spelling error Examples of invented spellings

    SubstitutionVowel substitution

    -The sound of /aI/ represented by the letter I  

    -The sound of /e/ represented by the letter a

    -The sound of /,/ approximated with the letter a

     because its name /e/ is similar to /,/

     Min (mine)

     Mak (make)

    Than (then)

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    49/65

      48

    -The sound /*/ approximated by representing it with

    the letter a

    Tack  (tuck)

    Consonant substitution

    -The sound /+/ represented with the letter f-The sound /+/ represented with the letter d  

    -The sound of /f/ approximated with the letter v

    -The sound of /v/ approximated with the letter w

    -The sound /t/ approximated with the letter d

    -The sound /d/ approximated with the letter t

    -The sound /-/ approximated with the letter n

    -The sound /s/ represented with the letter s because

    the sound is /s/

     Fersd (third) Ford (fourth)

    Give (gift)

    Wermit (vomit)

    Wend (went)

     Resterond (restaurant)

    Swin (swing)

     Nise (nice)

    Omission

    -Omission of silent vowel

    -Consonant cluster deletion

    -Omission of a vowel

    -Omission of a syllable

    -Consonant doubling

    befor (before),

    cocolate (chocolate)

     plese (please), set (seat)

    diffront (different)

    beautifull (beautiful), thiner (thinner)

    Insertion Thakeking (taking), bute (but)

    Transposition Siad (said), frist (first),

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    50/65

      49

    4516,(0 DF J:-"9--:%$ 1$) :+6':"1,:%$-

    In this chapter, the key findings from this research will be critically analysed in attempts

    to better understand Chinese ESL learner’s invented spellings by exploring their invented

    spellings made during writing summative assessments across the school year. This isdone in hopes to fill in the literature gap on Chinese ESL learner’s invented spellings.

    DG7 4%++%$ -6('':$* (00%0- +1)( /A *01)( %$( "5:')0($

    Based on Chan and Li’s (2000) research on the difference between English and

    Cantonese phonology, there were some spelling errors that overlapped with learner

    difficulties for native speakers of Cantonese. This supports the notion of taking into

    consideration of the language and orthographic differences when learning a second

    language (Wang & Geva, 2003). Furthermore, in a study conducted by Wang and Geva

    (2003), they found that Chinese ESL learners had difficulty in representing L2-specific

     phonemic elements in their spelling because it did not exist in their L1 phonology.

    Therefore it is important to take into account of these differences when teaching Chinese

    ESL learners.

    Based on the invented spellings identified from children’s writing summative assessment,

    some of the common errors can be matched to the three categories of consonants, voweland diphthongs, which were identified as pronunciation problems and difficulties faced

     by native speakers of Cantonese when learning English.

    Consonants

    Plosives

    Many of the children substituted the sounds /p, t, k/ for /b, d, g/ when they spelled words

    that contained these sounds. For example, blanket was spelled as plenket  (taken from the

    second mid-term writing summative assessment), where the /b/ sound was substituted

    with /p/. This is due to how in Cantonese there are no voiced syllable-final plosive sounds.

    Fricatives

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    51/65

      50

    These were common errors identified in children’s invented spellings from this research.

    Substitution was the main strategy used to help them spell when they encountered a

    difficult or unfamiliar sound.

    At the end of a word, the /f/ sound was substituted with /v/ so have was spelled as haf.

    Children would substitute /w/ for /v/, where vomit would be spelled as wermit   (taken

    from the second end-term assessment). Interestingly, they did not substitute /f/ for /v/ at

    the end of a word. They instead substituted /v/ for /f/.

    The /z/ sound does not exist in Cantonese, which if why is replaced with the voiceless /s/.

    So dizzy (taken from the first mid-term) is then spelled as desey, where /z/ is substituted

    for a /s/.

    The /+/ sound was substituted for either /d/ or /f/. This is evident with with being spelled

    as wife (taken from the first end-term assessment). Another example would be that

    spelled as dat. 

     Nasals, laterals and / $/

    In this circumstance, the sound /w/ was substituted for / $/, where whiteboard became

    ritboard , and wide became rad .

    Long vowels

    It has been said that Cantonese learners are unable to differentiate between the long and

    short vowel pairs. In this case, representing the sound of /u:/ with /'/. So food is then

    spelled as fud.

    In addition, there were other common errors where /e/ was represented with the letter a

     because the name of the letter sounds the same as the vowel that needs to be represented.

    This is similar to the letter sound /aI/ being represented by the letter I because of the letter

    name having the same name as the sound. This was a familiar occurrence in children’s

  • 8/17/2019 FullText (18)

    52/65

      51

    invented spellings, and it was because children began to realize that the conventional

    names of the vowels correspond to the sounds of the vowels (He & Wang, 2009).

    When the words contained the phonemes /*/, /-/, /i/ and /(/, they are often spelled

    according to the similar letter sounds of /a/, /n/, /I/ and /l/, respectively. Similarly, this is

    also done with the phonemes /, /, /+ /, and /Z / are spelled with similar letter sounds /I/,

    /d/ and /s/.

    The letter o was used to represent the sound /l/ because of its name o, which resulted in a

    close mid-back vowel, where castle becomes caso. The three phonemes /l/, /(/ and /o/ are

    all similar in regards to their place of articulation based on how the voiced lateral

    approximant /l/ becomes syllabic at the end of a word, and is then follo


Recommended